Moths and the ultimate death stare

Moths and the ultimate death stare

The survival of moths could be down to eyespots that give predators the ultimate death stare

Have you ever felt that a person in a portrait is watching you, their eyes following you about a room? This optical illusion is known as the Mona Lisa effect, after Leonardo da Vinci’s famously enigmatic painting.

When artists paint their sitter’s eyes with the pupils perfectly centred, no matter where visitors stand – to the left, right, or in front of the painting – eye contact is guaranteed.

Dr Hannah Rowland of the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology in Jena, Germany, and Dr John Skelhorn of the Centre for Behaviour and Evolution at Newcastle University, have conducted research that shows Nature seems to have hit upon the same idea. But in the animal kingdom, it can be a matter of life or death.

Many species of fish, butterflies, moths, praying mantids, and beetles have paired circular markings on their bodies that often appear to resemble eyes.

Eyespots can deflect a predator’s attention to a prey’s non-vital body parts (a prey is much more likely to survive a bite to its tail than its head). Eyespots can also be intimidating and deter predators from attacking at all. 

See also: Endangered butterfly has best UK summer in 150 years

most threatening

One explanation for this is that predators mistake eyespots for the eyes of their own predators. If this is the case, then eyespots that seem to look directly at them would be the most threatening.

Much like the portraits whose eyes seem to follow you around the room, eyespots might appear to maintain eye contact with predators no matter their vantage point.

On the other hand, if eyespots were displaced either to the left or right, they would only protect prey from predators approaching from that direction. 

Another explanation is that eyespots have nothing to do with eyes. Predators could be deterred not because eyespots look like eyes, but simply because they are patterns that stand out.

There are many conspicuous colours and patterns in the animal kingdom that are aversive to predators but look nothing like eyes – for example, the red and black patterns of ladybirds.

Humans might perceive eyespots to look like eyes, but that doesn’t necessarily mean that predators do too. They may simply see ‘scary’ conspicuous markings.

artificial moths

Rowland and Skelhorn tested the idea that forward-facing eyespots appear to gaze at predators by pitting artificial moths against newly hatched domestic chicks.

They created the moths by pinning paper triangles over mealworms – a favourite food of chicks. The paper triangles were printed with eyespots in one of three configurations: either perfectly concentric circles, or with the centre circle nudged to the right or left.

All of the prey were designed to be equally conspicuous to the predators. To the researchers, these appeared to gaze directly ahead or to one side and they wondered whether the direction of gaze affect the chicks’ motivation to attack.

slow to approach

They then built three miniature pathways to lead the chicks towards the moth. One led directly towards the prey, and the two others directed the chicks to approach the moth from either the left or right. The chicks were timed to see how long it took them to approach and attack each type of moth from each of the three directions. 

Chicks were slow to approach from the left when the moth’s eyespots were shifted to the left, and slow to approach from the right when the moth’s eyespots were shifted to the right.

However, when chicks approached these moths from the opposite direction, they quickly approached the moth and ate the mealworm. The chicks were slow to approach the moths with concentric circle eyespots from all three directions.

The results are consistent with the idea that the chicks perceived the artificial eyespots as eyes, and that eyespots are most effective when they appear to gaze at predators.

Eyespots that are concentric circles appear to stare at predators from a wider range of directions, just like the portraits that maintain eye contact from wherever you stand. That also probably explains why eyespots are so common in Nature. 

The research appears in Frontiers.

Image: A peacock butterfly with eyespots on its wings © Dr Hannah Rowland.

Leave a reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Aether: Issue 3 Feb 2023

Aether: Issue 2 Nov 2022

Aether: Issue 1 Aug 2022

Subscribe for free

Latest Testimonial

What a beautiful motto: Discoveries must be read and not just published. When I was contacted by Aether as a new digital service to share scientific and technological insights I had my doubts that this was really going to be according to what I call the “open source & makers’ spirit”: knowledge should be free and it is there to be shared.

Well, Aether is faithful to its motto and shares discoveries freely. It has been a pleasure to collaborate for the interview and subsequent article. It has been greatly self satisfying to see how the interview was professionally and truthfully redacted and then published. Sharing thoughts and sparks for discussions is fundamental to the progress of society. Your journal offers clarity and brevity and I believe it provides the sparks to ignite any reader whether academic or not into action.

Dr Maria-Cristina Ciocci
Co-founder and Manager of non-profit organisation De Creative STEM,GirlsInSTEM